U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Here's how you know

The .gov means it’s official. Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

The site is secure. A lock ( ) or https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Keyboard Navigation

Use these commands to navigate the primary menu and its sub menus via keyboard.
FunctionKey
Primary menu:
Sub menu:
Primary menu:Alt + o
Close menu:Esc
  • Agriculture and Food Security
  • Anti-Corruption
  • Conflict Prevention and Stabilization
  • Democracy, Human Rights, and Governance
  • Economic Growth and Trade
  • Environment, Energy, and Infrastructure
  • Gender Equality and Women's Empowerment
  • Global Health
  • Humanitarian Assistance
  • Innovation, Technology, and Research
  • Water and Sanitation
  • Burkina Faso
  • Central Africa Regional
  • Central African Republic
  • Côte d’Ivoire
  • Democratic Republic of the Congo
  • East Africa Regional
  • Power Africa
  • Republic of the Congo
  • Sahel Regional
  • Sierra Leone
  • South Africa
  • South Sudan
  • Southern Africa Regional
  • West Africa Regional
  • Afghanistan
  • Central Asia Regional
  • Indo-Pacific
  • Kyrgyz Republic
  • Pacific Islands
  • Philippines
  • Regional Development Mission for Asia
  • Timor-Leste
  • Turkmenistan
  • Bosnia and Herzegovina
  • North Macedonia
  • Central America and Mexico Regional Program
  • Dominican Republic
  • Eastern and Southern Caribbean
  • El Salvador
  • Middle East Regional Platform
  • West Bank and Gaza
  • Dollars to Results
  • Data Resources
  • Strategy & Planning
  • Budget & Spending
  • Performance and Financial Reporting
  • FY 2023 Agency Financial Report
  • Records and Reports
  • Budget Justification
  • Our Commitment to Transparency
  • Policy and Strategy
  • How to Work with USAID
  • Find a Funding Opportunity
  • Organizations That Work With USAID
  • Resources for Partners
  • Get involved
  • Business Forecast
  • Safeguarding and Compliance
  • Diversity, Equity, Inclusion, and Accessibility
  • Mission, Vision and Values
  • News & Information
  • Operational Policy (ADS)
  • Organization
  • Stay Connected
  • USAID History
  • Video Library
  • Coordinators
  • Nondiscrimination Notice and Civil Rights
  • Collective Bargaining Agreements
  • Disabilities Employment Program
  • Federal Employee Viewpoint Survey
  • Reasonable Accommodations
  • Urgent Hiring Needs
  • Vacancy Announcements
  • Search Search Search

Scaling Private Investment to Close the Development Financing Gap: Case Studies

This set of five case studies shows how donors and MDBs effectively engaged private institutional investors, leading to billions in private sector investment for development goals. The document also synthesizes key insights and recommendations on ways in which development actors can build on and replicate this nucleus of success and harness the scale and sophistication of global institutional investor assets.

Cover for Scaling Private Investment to Close the Development Financing Gap: Case Studies

development gap case study

ADB is committed to achieving a prosperous, inclusive, resilient, and sustainable Asia and the Pacific, while sustaining its efforts to eradicate extreme poverty.

Established in 1966, it is owned by 68 members—49 from the region..

Masatsugu Asakawa

  • Annual Reports
  • Policies and Strategies

ORGANIZATION

  • Board of Governors
  • Board of Directors
  • Departments and Country Offices

ACCOUNTABILITY

  • Access to Information
  • Accountability Mechanism
  • ADB and Civil Society
  • Anticorruption and Integrity
  • Development Effectiveness
  • Independent Evaluation
  • Administrative Tribunal
  • Ethics and Conduct
  • Ombudsperson

Strategy 2030

Annual meetings, adb supports projects in developing member countries that create economic and development impact, delivered through both public and private sector operations, advisory services, and knowledge support..

Asia and the Pacific's Climate Bank

ABOUT ADB PROJECTS

  • Projects & Tenders
  • Project Results and Case Studies

PRODUCTS AND SERVICES

  • Public Sector Financing
  • Private Sector Financing
  • Financing Partnerships
  • Funds and Resources
  • Economic Forecasts
  • Publications and Documents
  • Data and Statistics
  • Asia Pacific Tax Hub
  • Development Asia
  • ADB Data Library
  • Agriculture and Food Security
  • Climate Change
  • Digital Technology
  • Environment
  • Finance Sector
  • Fragility and Vulnerability
  • Gender Equality
  • Markets Development and Public-Private Partnerships
  • Regional Cooperation
  • Social Development
  • Sustainable Development Goals
  • Urban Development

REGIONAL OFFICES

  • European Representative Office
  • Japanese Representative Office | 日本語
  • North America Representative Office

LIAISON OFFICES

  • Pacific Liaison and Coordination Office
  • Pacific Subregional Office
  • Singapore Office

SUBREGIONAL PROGRAMS

  • Brunei, Indonesia, Malaysia, Philippines East ASEAN Growth Area (BIMP-EAGA)
  • Central Asia Regional Economic Cooperation (CAREC) Program
  • Greater Mekong Subregion (GMS) Program
  • Indonesia, Malaysia, Thailand Growth Triangle (IMT-GT)
  • South Asia Subregional Economic Cooperation (SASEC)

With employees from more than 60 countries, ADB is a place of real diversity.

Work with us to find fulfillment in sharing your knowledge and skills, and be a part of our vision in achieving a prosperous, inclusive, resilient, and sustainable asia and the pacific., careers and scholarships.

  • What We Look For
  • Career Opportunities
  • Young Professionals Program
  • Visiting Fellow Program
  • Internship Program
  • Scholarship Program

FOR INVESTORS

  • Investor Relations | 日本語
  • ADB Green and Blue Bonds
  • ADB Theme Bonds

INFORMATION ON WORKING WITH ADB FOR...

  • Consultants
  • Contractors and Suppliers
  • Governments
  • Executing and Implementing Agencies
  • Development Institutions
  • Private Sector Partners
  • Civil Society/Non-government Organizations

PROCUREMENT AND OUTREACH

  • Operational Procurement
  • Institutional Procurement
  • Business Opportunities Outreach

Closing Development Gaps: Challenges and Policy

Share this page.

Closing Development Gaps: Challenges and Policy

To close significant income and nonincome development gaps around the world, governments must go beyond policies that simply increase economic growth. They have to include policies that close nonincome development gaps directly.

  • http://hdl.handle.net/11540/1549

This study investigates income and nonincome development gaps that remain around the world. Closing these gaps requires policies that not only increase and sustain economic growth, but also directly close the nonincome development gaps. The authors analyze the extent to which human capital, infrastructure, financial sector development, governance and institutions, health and education, social protection, and external trade and finance play a role in closing these gaps.

  • Introduction
  • Key Dimensions of Development Gaps
  • Economic Growth as a Means to Closing Development Gaps
  • Closing Nonincome Development Gaps
  • The Role of Social Protection in Closing Income and Nonincome Development Gaps
  • The Role of External Trade and Finance in Closing Development Gaps
  • Concluding Remarks

Additional Details

Authors
Type
Series
Subjects
SKU
ISSN

Published Version

Brooks, Douglas H., Rana Hasan, Jong-Wha Lee, Hyun H. Son, and Juzhong Zhuang. 2010. "Closing Development Gaps: Challenges and Policy." Asian Development Review 27 (2): 1–28. https://doi.org/10.1142/S0116110510500071 .

  • More on social development and poverty

Also in this Series

  • Environmental, Social, and Governance Performance and Financial Impacts: Comparative Analysis of Companies in Asia
  • Exploring the Economic and Noneconomic Determinants of Investments in Renewable Energy
  • Debt Shocks and the Dynamics of Output and Inflation in Emerging Economies
  • ADB funds and products
  • Agriculture and natural resources
  • Capacity development
  • Climate change
  • Finance sector development
  • Gender equality
  • Governance and public sector management
  • Industry and trade
  • Information and Communications Technology
  • Private sector development
  • Regional cooperation and integration
  • Social development and protection
  • Urban development
  • Central and West Asia
  • Southeast Asia
  • The Pacific
  • China, People's Republic of
  • Lao People's Democratic Republic
  • Micronesia, Federated States of
  • Learning materials Guidelines, toolkits, and other "how-to" development resources
  • Books Substantial publications assigned ISBNs
  • Papers and Briefs ADB-researched working papers
  • Conference Proceedings Papers or presentations at ADB and development events
  • Policies, Strategies, and Plans Rules and strategies for ADB operations
  • Board Documents Documents produced by, or submitted to, the ADB Board of Directors
  • Financing Documents Describes funds and financing arrangements
  • Reports Highlights of ADB's sector or thematic work
  • Serials Magazines and journals exploring development issues
  • Brochures and Flyers Brief topical policy issues, Country Fact sheets and statistics
  • Statutory Reports and Official Records ADB records and annual reports
  • Country Planning Documents Describes country operations or strategies in ADB members
  • Contracts and Agreements Memoranda between ADB and other organizations

Subscribe to our monthly digest of latest ADB publications.

Follow adb publications on social media..

development gap case study

Case Study: Jamaica  

An example of how the growth of tourism in jamaica (a nee/) helps to reduce the development gap.

The development gap...

development gap case study

Jamaica is a tropical maritime island in the Caribbean Sea , situated about 145 km south of Cuba and 161 km southwest of Haiti.  

Jamaica is the fourth-largest island country in the Caribbean. It has a tropical climate with high temperatures throughout the year .

 Jamaica is famed for its beautiful sandy beaches and rich cultural heritage . It has excellent communications and is a popular destination for cruise ships.

Why visit Jamaica?

development gap case study

Growth of tourism

development gap case study

There was rapid growth in the number of visitor arrivals to Jamaica between 1994 and 2016. However, although visitor arrivals continued to increase in 2009, the change was not in line with previous years due to the recession, which reduced many people’s disposable income.

Total visitor expenditure increased from US$128,706,000 in 1975 to US$3,400,000,000 in 2019.

development gap case study

Benifitis of tourism

In 2014, tourism contributed to 24% of Jamaica's GDP, with this expected to rise to 32% by 2024

Income from tourism is $2 billion annually, with the taxes paid to the government contributing to reducing the development gap

Out of the 3.6 million annual visitors, 2.5 million spend $120 per day, with the remaining 1.1 million who arrive by cruise ship spending $70 per day

Employment (Social)

Tourism is the main source of employment in Jamaica, providing jobs for 200,000 people

Tourists spend money in shops or on services, further boosting the local economy

Those in employment learn new skills, which gives them improved prospects of better-paid jobs in the future

Overall improvement in the quality of life (access to services/ schools/ hospitals etc. -social improvements)

Environmental:

Conservation and landscaping projects provide jobs and opportunities

New water treatment plant at Logwood reduces pollution from hotels

Negril Marine Nature Park attracts tourists, bringing direct and indirect income

Community tourism and sustainable ecotourism is expanding in more isolated regions

development gap case study

Concerns of tourism

Socio-economic 

Northern tourist areas have benefitted from the tourist industry, for example Montego Bay, Ocho Rios are home to many wealthy Jamaicans with high-quality houses and a high standard of living (money to spend on more expensive goods). 

However large numbers of people live very nearby in relative poverty with a limited food supply and inadequate access to: fresh water; healthcare; education

Therefore, the effect of tourism is not evenly distributed in Jamaica. 

Environmental 

Footpath erosion 

Excessive waste, littering, hotels, cruise ship-water pollution. 

Harmful emissions from cruise ships- air pollutions

Damage to coral reefs

The multiplier effect 

How does tourism act as a regional multiplier in Jamaica?

More money is brought into Jamaica by tourists, which is then spent in shops, hotels, street vendors and services

The money generated increases the affluence of the area, with taxes contributing to the total economy

This prompts the government to invest in tourist areas, in order to: accommodate more people; ensure it is environmentally sustainable; increase attraction of the area

This in turn draws in more tourists, who spend more money - the cycle repeats (multiplier effect)

development gap case study

  • International
  • Education Jobs
  • Schools directory
  • Resources Education Jobs Schools directory News Search

Development Gap case study revision sheets - AQA, OCR and EdExcel with notes!

Development Gap case study revision sheets - AQA, OCR and EdExcel with notes!

Subject: Geography

Age range: 16+

Resource type: Worksheet/Activity

Geography and Sociology resource and lesson shop

Last updated

22 February 2018

  • Share through email
  • Share through twitter
  • Share through linkedin
  • Share through facebook
  • Share through pinterest

development gap case study

Tes paid licence How can I reuse this?

Get this resource as part of a bundle and save up to 50%

A bundle is a package of resources grouped together to teach a particular topic, or a series of lessons, in one place.

Geography Case study revision Paper 1 and Paper 2 - notes and task sheets

These sheets can be purchased seperately or as Paper 1 and Paper 2 bundles. The case studies work best for AQA Geography but also are used on EdExcel and OCR GCSE papers. The sheets cover all the content needed for each case study from the units that I deliver there are accompanying case study revision booklets with all of the notes to go with should you prefer them to be completed independently as revision or with support. The topics and case studies are; Paper 1; In the tectonics/restless earth pack includes grids on a rich and poor earthquake, a volcanic eruption, fold mountains and a Tsunami. Rich earthquake (Kobe, Japan), Poor earthquake (Sichuan, China), Volcano (Icelandic), Fold Mountains (The Andes) and Tsunami (Boxing day). Living world (ecosystems) includes, tropical rain forest (Malaysia), Rich desert (USA), Poor desert (Thar, India) and Temperate deciduous (Epping forest). Water on the Land/Rivers includes; Flooding in a MEDC (Tewkesbury), Flooding in a LEDC (Bangladesh), Hard Engineering (Three Gorges Dam) and Water management in the UK (Kielder Water). Paper 2; The Development Gap includes; Development Gap. The pack includes Haiti as an LEDC that suffers from physical inequality (Haiti Earthquake), Small scale aid youth empowerment in Uganda, Medium scale aid, Action Aid in Kolkata, Large scale Aid - the Cahora Bassa dam, Mozambique and inequality in the EU. Urban environments includes; sustainable transport in London and sustainable living in Curitiba, squatter settlements in Kibera, Kenya and managing CBD's including UDC's (Newcastle upon Tyne), City Challenge (Manchester) ,Sustainable communities (New Islington) and Managing a city sustainability (Newcastle upon Tyne). Tourism includes; the pack includes questioning sheets and resource interpretation on Blackpool (UK coastal resort including Butler model), The Galapagos Islands (Ecotourism), Antarctica (Extreme Tourism) and Jamaica (Tourism in the Tropics).

AQA Geography Paper 2 case study revision pack

Students complete the case studies with probing questions for the topic on The Development Gap, Urban environments and Tourism. The Development Gap includes; Development Gap. The pack includes Haiti as an LEDC that suffers from physical inequality (Haiti Earthquake), Small scale aid youth empowerment in Uganda, Medium scale aid, Action Aid in Kolkata, Large scale Aid - the Cahora Bassa dam, Mozambique and inequality in the EU. Urban environments includes; sustainable transport in London and sustainable living in Curitiba, squatter settlements in Kibera, Kenya and managing CBD's including UDC's (Newcastle upon Tyne), City Challenge (Manchester) ,Sustainable communities (New Islington) and Managing a city sustainability (Newcastle upon Tyne). Tourism includes; the pack includes questioning sheets and resource interpretation on Blackpool (UK coastal resort including Butler model), The Galapagos Islands (Ecotourism), Antarctica (Extreme Tourism) and Jamaica (Tourism in the Tropics). Students complete the sheets for revision alongside the booklets included, the questions are challenging and related to the exam specification/question demands. This can be a lesson revision activity or a stand alone weekly set of home work tasks.

Your rating is required to reflect your happiness.

It's good to leave some feedback.

Something went wrong, please try again later.

This resource hasn't been reviewed yet

To ensure quality for our reviews, only customers who have purchased this resource can review it

Report this resource to let us know if it violates our terms and conditions. Our customer service team will review your report and will be in touch.

Not quite what you were looking for? Search by keyword to find the right resource:

  • 0 Shopping Cart

Internet Geography

How can the growth of tourism reduce the development gap?

Tunisia Case Study

An image showing the remains of Carthage, Tunisia

How can the growth of tourism reduce the development gap? Tunisia Case Study

Tunisia is a North African country bordering the Mediterranean Sea and the Sahara Desert. It has a favourable climate with hot summers and mild winters. It has a range of cultural and historic attractions including the ancient city of Carthage and several Star Wars locations were filmed in the country. Other attractions include the Mediterranean beaches and the Sahara desert. It is accessible via good connections with Europe.  

An image showing the remains of Carthage, Tunisia

The ancient city of Carthage, Tunisia

How has tourism in Tunisia grown?

Tunisia experienced a year-on-year average growth rate of 1.14 % for the time period 1976 to 2017 . Since the recession in 2009, there has been an almost exponential growth in tourism in terms of the contribution of travel services to commercial service imports.

How has Tunisia reduced the development gap?  

Over 370,00 jobs have been created in the tourism sector, boosting incomes and increasing the movement of money within the economy. The development of coastal resorts has benefited a range of local businesses including taxis, restaurants, shops and the construction industry. The growth of tourism has boosted the agricultural sector as demand for food has increased with tourist numbers. Due to the economic boost provided by tourism the government has been able to invest more money into education and health care. This investment has resulted in improved literacy rates and life expectancy .  

Internet Geography Plus

Premium Resources

Please support internet geography.

If you've found the resources on this page useful please consider making a secure donation via PayPal to support the development of the site. The site is self-funded and your support is really appreciated.

Related Topics

Use the images below to explore related GeoTopics.

How can the growth of tourism reduce the development gap? Jamaica Case Study

Topic home, what is nigeria’s location and importance, share this:.

  • Click to share on Twitter (Opens in new window)
  • Click to share on Facebook (Opens in new window)
  • Click to share on Pinterest (Opens in new window)
  • Click to email a link to a friend (Opens in new window)
  • Click to share on WhatsApp (Opens in new window)
  • Click to print (Opens in new window)

If you've found the resources on this site useful please consider making a secure donation via PayPal to support the development of the site. The site is self-funded and your support is really appreciated.

Search Internet Geography

Home

Latest Blog Entries

2024 AQA GCSE Geography exam analysis data

Pin It on Pinterest

  • Click to share
  • Print Friendly
  • Find Flashcards
  • Why It Works
  • Tutors & resellers
  • Content partnerships
  • Teachers & professors
  • Employee training

Brainscape's Knowledge Genome TM

Entrance exams, professional certifications.

  • Foreign Languages
  • Medical & Nursing

Humanities & Social Studies

Mathematics, health & fitness, business & finance, technology & engineering, food & beverage, random knowledge, see full index.

Geography > Development Gap Case Study - Development Projects > Flashcards

Development Gap Case Study - Development Projects Flashcards

What is the large scale aid project?

The Cahora Bassa Dam, Mozambique

What are the environmental impacts of the large scale project?

Massive environmental damage. River flow is low. Shrimp fishing industry has been almost destroyed.

Give some facts about the large scale aid project.

Portuguese government began construction in the 1960’s. Civil war halted development and damaged infrastructure. Construction finished in 1997. Largest HEP scheme in Southern Africa. One of three dams constructed in the Zambezi Basin. Top-down development.

Who has benefitted from the large scale aid project?

1% of Mozambique’s rural homes.

Most of the power is sold to South Africa.

How successful was the large scale project?

Highly unsuccessful: • only 1% of the population benefit from it • cause of major environmental damage • destroyed shrimp fishing industry and jobs

Because of this, it has slowed development despite its great potential.

What is the medium scale project?

Give some facts about the medium scale project.

WaterAid was established in 1981. They support countries that lack suitable sanitation, safe water and hygiene education to promote development and greater living standards.

How has Water Aid supported Madagascar?

Reached 61,000 people with safe water. Gave 110,000 people improved sanitation. Made sure that practical skills and technologies existed in the community. Schools and health centres were given choices about the best solutions for their problems. Simple solutions (e.g. covering stored water, hand washing) were encouraged.

How has WaterAid supported Ethiopia?

Reached 113,000 people with safe water. Gave 123,000 people improved sanitation. Coordinated a country water, sanitation and hygiene coalition which promotes water and sanitation across the country.

What is the small scale project?

Community Youth Empowerment Programme, Uganda

Give some facts about the small scale project.

It works to improve water quality and education in small Ugandan communities. Bottom-up development.

What are the short term benefits of the small scale project?

Instant access to uncontaminated, protected water.

What are the long term benefits of the small scale project?

Younger generation are educated on environmental health, concerns and energy conservation methods. Availability of clean water can be prolonged. Promotes sustainable development. Future skilled workforce.

Geography (26 decks)

  • Restless Earth
  • Population Case Study - China's one child policy
  • Population Case Study - Transmigration in Indonesia
  • Population Case Study - Ageing Population in East Devon
  • Population Case Study - Migration from Poland to the UK
  • Living World Case Study - Papua New Guinea Rainforest
  • Living World Case Study - Threats to the Malaysian Rainforest
  • Living World
  • Living World Case Study - Sustainable Management of the Malaysian Rainforest
  • Living World Case Study - The Sonoran Desert (MEDC)
  • Restless Earth Case Study - Nyiragongo Volcano
  • Restless Earth Case Study - Boxing Day Tsunami
  • Living World Case Study - Epping Forest
  • Development Gap
  • Development Gap Case Study - Development Projects
  • Development Gap Case Study - Levels Of Development
  • Globalisation
  • Globalisation Case Study - Toyota
  • Population Case Study - Migration to the EU
  • Globalisation Case Study - China as an Economic Giant
  • Restless Earth Case Study - Yellowstone
  • Restless Earth Case Study - The Andes
  • Restless Earth Case Study - Eyjafjallajökull
  • Restless Earth Case Study - Kobe
  • Restless Earth Case Study - Haiti
  • Corporate Training
  • Teachers & Schools
  • Android App
  • Help Center
  • Law Education
  • All Subjects A-Z
  • All Certified Classes
  • Earn Money!

Thank you for visiting nature.com. You are using a browser version with limited support for CSS. To obtain the best experience, we recommend you use a more up to date browser (or turn off compatibility mode in Internet Explorer). In the meantime, to ensure continued support, we are displaying the site without styles and JavaScript.

  • View all journals
  • Explore content
  • About the journal
  • Publish with us
  • Sign up for alerts
  • Open access
  • Published: 13 September 2024

Children’s limited tooling ability in a novel concurrent tool use task supports the innovation gap

  • Jennifer A. D. Colbourne 1 ,
  • Alice M. I. Auersperg 1 &
  • Sarah R. Beck 2  

Scientific Reports volume  14 , Article number:  21374 ( 2024 ) Cite this article

Metrics details

  • Human behaviour

School-aged children have consistently shown a surprising developmental lag when attempting to innovate solutions to tool use tasks, despite being capable of learning to solve these problems from a demonstrator. We suggest that this “innovation gap” arises from tool tasks with more complex spatial relations. Following Fragaszy and Mangalam’s new tooling theory, we predicted that innovating a new “sticker slide” task should be more challenging when two tools need to be used at the same time (concurrently) rather than one at a time (sequentially), despite the similarity of the other task elements. In line with previous work, both versions of the task were challenging for all ages of children (4–9 years) that we tested. However, the youngest group showed particularly extreme difficulties, which was marked by not a single child innovating the concurrent version. Although success significantly increased with age, even the oldest group failed to reach 50% success on the concurrent version of the task, whereas the majority of the two older groups could solve the sequential version. Thus, in this first study of concurrent tool use in children, we found support for the prediction that increasing the complexity of spatial relations in tooling exacerbates the innovation gap.

Introduction

Children may have all of the necessary motor skills to tie their shoelaces or braid their hair, but they are unlikely to learn to do so without repeated close observation or explicit instruction. There is a divide between the ability to engage in certain behaviours, and the capability of conceiving of them a priori. Often, it is not until certain actions have been observed or learnt from others that the possibility for action is realised. Accordingly, this is why young children benefit from instruction and guidance; complex behaviours are in a sense “jump-started” through social learning, though they may have eventually been independently discovered. Such independent problem solving, in the absence of social learning or a model solution, is known as innovation 1 .

The gap between innovating a solution unaided and being capable of learning it can vary substantially, and during development has been found to be strikingly large in the area of object relations. Bates and colleagues discovered that 10-month-old infants could only successfully solve means-end tasks if the reaching object was visibly in contact with the reward, even when the motor actions to succeed were the same in no-contact and contact tasks (e.g., placing a toy in the middle of a hoop to be pulled versus placing the toy in contact with the inside of the hoop) 2 . They concluded that the link between the two objects needed to be visually apparent in order for children to reach the conclusion that one could affect the other; the infants were incapable of perceiving a spatial relationship between the objects that did not yet exist. Subsequent research has showed that children can begin to successfully employ a reaching tool with a spatial gap around 18 months of age, although the majority could only do so after a demonstration or hint 3 , 4 , 5 . It is not until approximately 30 months that the majority of children systematically innovate the solution without adult aid 5 , 6 .

In a series of tool use studies led by Beck with young school-aged children, another surprising innovation gap in the area of object relations was discovered. This work was initially inspired by the discovery that Betty the New Caledonian crow ( Corvus moneduloides ) could innovate a hook tool by bending piece of wire in order to retrieve an out-of-reach reward inside of a vertical tube 7 , leading to the question of when such an ability emerged in children. In Beck and colleagues’ version of the task, children aged 3 to 11 years were presented with a tube in which lay a handled-basket containing a prize and given a pipe cleaner and a distractor item 8 . Surprisingly, it was not until 8 years of age that the majority of children were successful at innovating a hook tool, despite nearly all children being able to correctly select a pre-made hook tool to solve the problem, and easily able to make the requisite tool after a demonstration 8 . In subsequent experiments, various modifications to the hook task were made in order to discover which factors might influence success, but to no effect. For example, children were given experience with the bending properties of the pipe cleaner 8 , 9 , 10 , exploration time with the materials 10 , 11 , clear ownership over the tool 12 , instructions to try to make something 9 , prompts to change strategy 11 and drawing attention from the experimenter by having a puppet present the task 9 . Even experience making another pipe cleaner tool 9 and building a hook tool made from wooden pieces 13 failed to increase success rates. Moreover, children’s personality traits also did not have an effect 14 . The failure to innovate on the hook task was replicated with indigenous children in South Africa, Australia and the Congo, whose developmental and cultural experiences with object manipulation vary widely from the “WEIRD” (Western, Educated, Industrialized, Rich, and Democratic 15 ) populations tested by Beck and colleagues, indicating that the underlying challenge may be a cognitive one, rather than environmental 10 , 16 , 17 (but see 12 , wherein older children visiting a museum showed higher success rates).

One suggestion is that the issue may be one of executive function, that is the cognitive abilities related to the control of behaviour. The nature of the hook task as an “ill-structured problem,” i.e., a problem lacking a clear path from start to goal, has been particularly noted as necessitating a certain degree of domain-general executive ability 1 , 9 , 11 , 18 , 19 . However, executive function and divergent thinking tasks, involving inhibition, attentional flexibility, hierarchical structuring, and working memory, have not found higher-scoring children to be better hook innovators 14 , 18 , 20 , 21 (although there is evidence some of these factors may contribute to learning after a demonstration 20 , 21 ). The problem also does not appear to be one of causal understanding, as most children can immediately select the correct tool and/or copy the manufacture of the correct tool 8 , 9 , 11 , and often innovation success rates will increase in older children after simply being shown a pre-made hook tool 19 , 21 , 22 . Remarkably, though, when given pre-made hooks that don’t fit inside the tube, either due to being oversized or having a curl at the end, children still perform poorly on the hook task 16 , 23 , indicating that it may be that the difficulty lies in the reshaping of the tool itself.

Another tool reshaping task given to children, in which a folded wire must be unbent to push out a reward through a horizontal tube (“Unbending Task”), has been found to be easier than the hook bending task, in that it is solved on average at a younger age (7 years to reach 50% compared to 9.5 years; see Fig.  1 . for predicted percentage of success based on data from previous research) 9 , 11 , 24 , 25 . Even with this simpler task, a similar innovation gap pattern is still apparent with higher rates of success (e.g., > 75%) only appearing late in childhood after age 10. Furthermore, children perform at significantly higher rates on this task and the hook task when they can make functional tools by adding, detaching or subtracting pieces, rather than using wire to shape a tool 26 . Building a wooden hook out of dowel pieces has also been found to be as difficult, or nearly as difficult, as reshaping a hook 13 , 25 , 27 . Thus, it may be something fundamental about making a new tool shape that is particularly difficult for young children. When considering a new tool form, one must conceive of how its new spatial relations could function to solve the problem at hand, and apply this to its creation. The challenges presented by this type of complex spatial reasoning ability could thus very well be at heart of the tool innovation issue.

figure 1

The innovation gap in two tool reshaping tasks. Each colour represents a study, each point represents an experimental condition, and sample size for each experimental condition is reflected in point size. The black line represents predicted values, weighted by sample size. The “hook task” represents only conditions in which a wire had to be reshaped; results from conditions in which children were shown a premade hook tool template before attempting the task were not included (as any subsequent tool made was not strictly innovated). More information on the experiments and data included in the graphs can be found in the open access repository.

Therefore, we created a tool use innovation task that eliminated tool manufacture but retained the necessity of considering novel object spatial relations in order to innovate a solution. According to Fragaszy and Mangalam’s tooling theory, it is the spatial relations involved in the act of tooling that determines the task’s complexity 28 . This is because using a grasped tool to create a mechanical effect (“tooling”) transforms the body into a body-plus-object system, in what appears to be a neurocognitively distinct phenomenon in which the body schema is extended while using a tool 29 , 30 . The grasped tool fundamentally alters how the actor can act in the environment as the locus of control shifts from the hand to the end-effector (distalization) and the degrees of freedom for physical movement are redistributed. Furthermore, the actor must simultaneously monitor the relationship between their grasping hand and the tool, and the effective end of the tool (the end-effector) and its mechanical interaction with the target 28 . When two tools are used in both hands at the same time, this management doubles, making it more difficult to use two tools concurrently compared to one after the other sequentially. Even without the use of tools, concurrent bimanual reaching takes longer than unimanual reaching (“two target cost”), which has been suggested to be due to the attentional demands of monitoring two objects simultaneously during sensorimotor coordination of the limbs 31 , 32 , 33 , 34 . Intriguingly, in tasks that require reaching bimanually for two items each placed either near or far to the participant, older children (7–10 years) exhibit a limited version of the approach taken by adults, in that movement time is increased only in congruent conditions (near/near, far/far) but not incongruent (near/far), which has been suggested to be due to the increasing sensorimotor integration that takes place during this transitional period of development 34 . They also differ from younger children (4–6 years), in that older children exhibit more adult-like synchronization, whereas younger children’s bimanual reaches are more asynchronous with weaker coupling, with the limbs almost sequential in their movements 34 .

Accordingly, we hypothesized that a concurrent tool use task should be more difficult to innovate than the same task with a sequential solution. We expected this difference should be more apparent in younger school-aged children, considering the innovation lag previously observed in other developmental tool innovation studies; therefore, we tested a range of children during the period of change in their tool innovation ability (4–9 years). To this end, we designed the “sticker slide” box, which has a gate at the bottom requiring a sturdy dowel to open it, that, when closed, blocks a smaller hole on top, in which can be inserted a thin straw to dislodge the reward from a platform inside (see Fig.  2 ). In the sequential version, the gate stays open after the dowel is inserted, allowing the apparatus to be solved in two discrete steps, whereas in the concurrent version, due to the addition of a small piece of sponge, the gate must be continually kept open with the dowel in one hand in order to probe inside with the thinner tool in the other hand (for a demonstration of each task version, see supplementary video online). According to tooling theory, the spatial relations of the concurrent version of the task should be more difficult to manage, and thus more difficult to innovate, despite each element of the apparatus remaining the same in both versions (opening the gate with one tool, dislodging the ball with the other). We expected that children who failed to innovate either solution would still be physically capable of completing the task after a demonstration and/or instruction from the experimenter, as is typical of other tool use tasks.

figure 2

The sticker slide puzzle box. ( a ) Side view with box measurements, ( b ) high angle view with labels for essential components, ( c ) prize ball, wooden dowel tool, and thin straw tool with measurements.

Innovation success

We ran a logistic GLMM with success as an outcome variable (fail/success) with age and an interaction between task condition (sequential/concurrent) and task order (group 1/group 2) as fixed effects, and participant identity as a random effect. Age was z-transformed to increase interpretability of the coefficients 35 . The total number of observations was n  = 186 (representing 98 children). We compared the full model to a null model lacking the predictor of interest (condition) using a likelihood ratio test 36 . We found that the model containing condition significantly predicted success (χ 2 (2) = 34.76, p  < 0.001). The interaction between condition and task order was also significant (χ 2 (1) = 12.588, p  < 0.001). These results were driven by the group that was given the sequential condition first, which was significantly more successful on the following concurrent condition (35.42%) compared to the group that was given the concurrent condition first (15.22%; z  = 2.93, p  = 0.018), while both groups had similar rates of success on the sequential condition (50.00% and 52.17%; z  = 0.59, p  = 0.937; see Fig.  3 ). Looking at the groups separately, the concurrent task was significantly more difficult than sequential for the group given the concurrent task first ( z  = 3.56, p  = 0.002), but not for the group given sequential first ( z  = 2.18, p  = 0.130). Age was significant as a main effect (see Table 1 ). Diagnostics of the model assumptions for collinearity, stability, distribution of the random effect and overdispersion did not reveal any cause for concern.

figure 3

Predicted probability of success by age and task order (group). Shaded areas represent 95% confidence intervals.

The comparative difficulty of the concurrent version compared to the sequential version of the task can also be seen by the fact that none of the 5-year-old children ( n  = 33) managed to solve the former, compared to 16.13% of these same children successfully solving the sequential condition (see Fig.  4 ). Similarly, the 7-year-old ( n  = 32) and 9-year-old ( n  = 33) groups performed better on the sequential condition compared to the concurrent condition (7-year-old group: 60% vs 34.38%; 9-year-old group: 77.42% vs 43.33%); the age effect is also apparent from these numbers. Unlike the two older groups, the majority of the 5-year-olds required help during the pre-experience to familiarize them with knocking the ball off of the plinth (5-year-old group: 64%, 7-year-old group: 19%, 9-year-old group: 15%). More 5-year-olds also gave up trying to solve the puzzle in both the sequential and the concurrent versions (sequential: 16/25 children that gave up, concurrent: 15/27 children that gave up). For those children that did not succeed on the experiment and were given a demonstration afterwards, all of the 9-year-olds (18/18) were able to solve the puzzle by themselves, while 25% (8/32) of the 5-year-olds and 38% (8/21) of 7-year-olds needed a hint in order to solve the puzzle, and 41% (13/32) of 5-year-olds and 5% of 7-year-olds (1/21) needed actual physical assistance with one of the tools in order to succeed. Three children (one in the 5-year-old group, two in the 7-year-old group) never succeeded.

figure 4

Success on each version of the sticker slide by age group.

Of the children who were unsuccessful, 64% (29/45) tried only one tool on the sequential condition and 39% (27/70) on the concurrent condition; 71% (32/45) tried only the top hole and 2% (1/45) tried only the bottom hole in the sequential condition, and 49% ( 3 4/70) tried only the top hole and 13% (9/70) tried only the bottom hole in the concurrent condition (see Table 2 for percentages by age group). In the sequential condition, 7% (3/45) of unsuccessful children succeeded in pushing the gate open but failed to solve the puzzle, and in the concurrent condition, 27% (19/70) pushed the gate open but failed. For the children given sequential first (group 1) that passed the sequential condition but subsequently failed the concurrent condition, 62.5% (5/8) discovered the gate in the concurrent condition and 37.5% (3/8) did not. We found 2% of children innovated the correct solution for the concurrent condition but failed to retrieve the ball within the time limit (one in the 5-year-old group and one in the 7-year-old group). An additional 6% were close to innovating the correct solution and engaged in concurrent tool use but did so with both tools in the bottom hole, where the straw was unable to reach the ball (three from the 7-year-old group and three from the 9-year-old group).

Solving time

In order to investigate whether the concurrent condition solution took longer to innovate than the sequential condition solution, we ran a model to see if condition predicted time to success. As only children who succeeded were included in this analysis, and no 5-year-old children solved the concurrent condition, we did not include this younger age group in the model so that the five children that solved sequential did not bias the data (note that on average, the 5-year-old group in sequential performed nearly twice as slow as the older children in both conditions, M  = 109.6 s vs M  = 61.2 s). Consequently, the total number of observations was n  = 66 (representing 45 children). We ran a LMM with time to success as an outcome variable with age and an interaction between condition (sequential/concurrent) and task order (group 1/group2) as fixed effects, and participant identity as a random effect. Age was z-transformed to increase the interpretability of the coefficients, and time to success was log-transformed to adjust for positive skew. We compared the full model to a null model lacking the predictor of interest (condition) using a likelihood ratio test. We found that the model containing condition significantly predicted time to success (χ 2 (2) = 14.12, p  < 0.001). The interaction between condition and task order was also significant (χ 2 (1) = 14.12, p  < 0.001), but not age ( t (39.21) = 0.34, p  = 0.736). The semi-partial R 2 of the two interacting variables accounted for 16.9% of the variance of the response. The LMM model assumptions of normality and homoscedasticity of residuals, normal distribution of the random effect, non-multicollinearity and stability were met.

The interaction in the solving time model was driven by the fact that group 1, which received sequential first, took longer to solve the sequential condition compared to concurrent ( M  = 87.89 s vs M  = 65.29 s), while group 2, which received the concurrent condition first, took longer to solve concurrent ( M  = 62.86 s) compared to sequential ( M  = 36.04 s) (see Supplementary Fig. S1 online). In other words, whichever condition was given first took longer to solve than the second for both groups. Note that the results are somewhat biased in that more children solved sequential ( n  = 42) than concurrent ( n  = 24); while roughly equal numbers passed sequential in group 1 ( n  = 19) and group 2 ( n  = 23), more children in group 1 passed concurrent ( n  = 17) compared to group 2 ( n  = 7).

Dislodging time

During data collection, it was observed that there could be substantial time spent between finding the solution to the puzzle and actually dislodging the prize ball. Measuring the time from first contact of the thin straw tool with the ball and the ball being knocked off the plinth, we conducted an exploratory post hoc model to see if condition affected this dislodging time, to see if it might be the case that the motor demands of using two tools concurrently with two hands are more difficult than when using only one hand to manipulate one tool. As with the success time model, only children who actually solved the puzzle were included in this model, so the data was limited only to the children in the 7-year-old group and the 9-year-old group (the five 5-year-old children in sequential were removed again to avoid biasing the data). Furthermore, a two-part hurdle-style model had to be employed as roughly half of the children immediately knocked the ball off of the plinth, resulting in an uneven distribution due to excess zeroes. The time to dislodge data was log transformed due to positive skew. Full-null comparisons revealed neither the zero part (χ 2 (1) = 0.440, p  = 0.507) nor the remainder of the model part (χ 2 (1) = 0.012, p  = 0.913) were significant. This is reflected in the fact that roughly equal numbers of children immediately solved the puzzle as those that required more time, in both the sequential (20 vs 22) and concurrent conditions (10 vs 14). Similarly, the amount of time taken to dislodge the ball was similar in both conditions (sequential: 20.68 s; concurrent: 21.57 s). Diagnostics for the assumptions of both models were acceptable, however the random effect of participant identity was overfitted.

Similar to prior work, we found a lag in children’s ability to innovate. Even though the sequential version of the sticker slide task could technically be solved without a deeper understanding of the function of the gate and tools (i.e., by inserting the various tools in the holes until the gate opened/ the prize ball was dislodged), not even a quarter of the youngest group was successful (compared to over three-quarters of the oldest group). Remarkably, not a single child of the younger group succeeded in innovating the concurrent version. Even after a demonstration by the experimenter, most of the 5-year-old group required help in the form of a hint or even physical assistance to succeed. This was needed by none of the oldest age group. This is in contrast to the hook manufacture tasks, where some children as young as three years can make a hook and solve the puzzle after a single demonstration. The concurrent condition was more difficult to innovate than the sequential condition for the older children, as the oldest group of 9-year-olds only reached a success rate of 43.33%, which is remarkably lower than the success rate of the tool reshaping tasks for that age. Their success rate for sequential, however, is much more comparable (77.42%) to the hook and horizontal tube tasks.

For the older two age groups, the amount of time required to innovate and successfully solve the puzzle in both conditions only depended on which condition had been given first. And so, whereas the concurrent condition may be harder to innovate, for those children that do innovate, there does not appear to be a difference in solving time between the two conditions. Similarly, there did not appear to be a difference between conditions when looking at the time to knock the ball off the plinth, after contact was made with a tool. These results hint that the chief difficulty is making the innovation itself, rather than a more physical element, such as motor coordination. Of the children who tried only one hole, the large majority focused only on the top hole rather than the bottom. It is possible that this is because the top hole was directly in the children’s line of sight when seated, and/or that the children were influenced by the location of the hole in the pre-experience box and persisted in this approach due to a kind of functional fixedness. Considering two-thirds of the children who were unsuccessful on the sequential condition only tried one tool, this further suggests persistence with a single approach as a cause of failure.

However, for both models, the data was limited to only the successful children, of which there were many more in the sequential than concurrent condition, and also resulted in no representation from the youngest group. Considering the difficulty this group had completing the concurrent version after the demonstration, potential differences in motor demands between simultaneous and concurrent tool use for younger children cannot be dismissed. It is also possible that the difference is relatively small, requiring a more sensitive methodology to measure. For example, in one key pressing study, the difference between unimanual and bimanual responses in children differed only by milliseconds 37 . Interestingly, the researchers found bimanual responses took significantly longer than when responding to the same stimulus unimanually, but only once the children reached six years of age 37 . This phenomenon, known as “bimanual cost,” also occurs in adults, and has been suggested to be caused by inhibitory networks in the corpus callosum that slow the response of each hemisphere when coupling bimanual responses 37 , 38 . Consequently, its absence in younger children suggests that this rudimentary form of interhemispheric communication supporting bimanual movement has not yet developed 37 . Similarly, when reaching to grasp with both arms simultaneously, compared to older children (7–10 years), younger children (4–6 years) show immature patterns of bimanual coordination, with each limb moving in a more sequential manner, resulting in weaker spatial and temporal coupling 34 , 39 . Bimanual tasks that require coordinating two distinct actions (role differentiated bimanual movements) also show marked differences in movement time and smoothness between 5–6 years and 7–9 years 40 .

The gradual maturation of the corpus callosum has been suggested to be an important factor in the development of both interhemispheric integration and bimanual coordination 40 , 41 . The callosal isthmus, which connects temporo-parietal areas associated the understanding of spatial relations and language, exhibits rapid growth between 7 and 11 years 42 , as well as functional-structural reorganization and refinement during age 6 to 8 43 . Immature bimanual coordination may explain why some of the younger children in our study needed physical assistance with one of the tools in order to eventually dislodge the prize. However, considering there were only two cases of children correctly innovating the solution but failing to dislodge the ball in time in the concurrent condition, it does not seem likely that the issue was one of bimanual coordination per se. However, lack of sensorimotor integration between the two hemispheres certainly could impede the children’s ability to consider prospective concurrent motor solutions. It is certainly intriguing, and likely a fruitful area for subsequent research, that eight years of age has been argued to represent a developmental milestone in motor efficacy and sensorimotor integration 34 , 44 , 45 , which is around the same time children have been found to improve at tool innovation tasks (as shown in Fig.  1 ).

This is the first study to look explicitly at the innovation of concurrent tool use in children. Previously, there have been a few studies of sequential tool use in children under six years of age, all of which involve using a tool to obtain a different tool that can access a reward, with success rates ranging from 5.1 to 37.5%, depending on the amount of pre-experience the children had with the tool elements 46 , 47 , 48 . In line with these low success rates, the youngest group in our study (5-year-olds) was only 16% successful. Even when requiring only one tool, solving a puzzle with two different actions is also difficult for older children; in a task requiring a rod to push an object inside a box then rake it out (“chimney task”), just over half of the 7-year-olds tested were correct, whereas on a task requiring the use of a rod first as a lever to bring the object closer, then next extracting the object (“sloping task”), it was not until 10½ years of age that just over half were successful 49 . Clearly, using multiple tools or the same tool for multiple functions is a developmental challenge.

This type of tool use, referred to as associative tool use in the non-human animal literature 50 , 51 , is not only difficult for young humans. For example, the use of the same tool for different functions (multi-functional tool) has only been repeatedly observed in chimpanzees ( Pan troglodytes ) 52 . Although there are more cases of sequential tool use, it is limited to only a few highly intelligent animal species (e.g., great apes 53 , 54 ); tufted capuchin monkeys ( Sapajus spp. ) 55 ; Goffin’s cockatoos ( Cacatua goffiniana ) 56 ; New Caledonian crows ( Corvus moneduloides ) 57 ; rooks ( Corvus frugilegus ) 58 . To date, there are no studies with animals engaged in concurrent tool use, and Fragaszy and Mangalam have hypothesized that simultaneously tooling with two grasped objects is beyond the capacity of any animal species 28 .

In comparison to associative tool use, success rates of single-step tool use tasks are typically much higher and within the capability of children under three years of age (e.g. spoon feeding, hammering pegs, raking in an out of reach object, as well as various probing tasks in the great ape tool test battery (GATTeB) 5 , 59 , 60 , 61 . Notably, the one task that children found especially difficult to innovate in the GATTeB was “nuthammer,” in which a child needed to use a clay hammer to crack open a plastic nut; the experimenters thought this task was particularly demanding because it involved multiple steps and the coordination of several objects (hammer, anvil and nut) 61 , 62 . This type of associative tool use, also referred to as composite tool use 50 , 51 , is similarly challenging for animals. The use of a hammer and anvil is limited to a few primate species (e.g. chimpanzees ( Pan troglodytes ) 51 ; long-tailed macaques ( Macaca fascicularis ) 63 ; capuchin monkeys 64 ). In this case, two spatial relations need to be managed in a sequence (nut-to-anvil, hammer-to-nut). The only other comparable tool use task to date is a laboratory study with Goffin’s cockatoos, in which a ball needs to be placed inside a box, then maneuvered with a stick in order to collapse a platform (“golf club task”) 65 . Success on this task depends on an external-concurrent interaction, in which two objects are simultaneously used as the positioning and movement of the ball is controlled by the stick 65 . This task is the closest in spatial relations management to the concurrent tool use task of the sticker slide, and in parallel to our own results, only three of the cockatoos were capable of consistently solving the task, out of the eleven capable of using tools to solve simpler tasks.

It can thus be seen, in both children and animals, that the increased number of spatial relations as well as the temporal order of managing these relations sequentially or concurrently affects the success rate of tool use innovation tasks. As predicted by tooling theory, we found that innovating concurrent tool use was considerably more difficult than sequential tool use. In the sequential tool task, it is possible for the solution to be fortuitously discovered while exploring by inserting dowel in the bottom hole, without necessitating a deeper understanding of the function of the gate or the spatial relations involved. In contrast, the concurrent tool task requires considering the prospective spatial relations concerning the gate and the provided objects, in addition to keeping the gate open to allow access to the plinth while simultaneously monitoring both tools. The higher success rates of the group that received sequential first, however, indicates that discovery of the gate’s affordances may have facilitated performance in the concurrent condition. Still, it is telling that although some of the youngest children were successful on the sequential version, none of them were able to solve the concurrent condition, despite those in the group given sequential first ostensibly realising the gate could open. Moreover, nearly a third of children in the concurrent condition pushed the gate open and still failed, indicating that at least some had an idea of how the gate functioned. It is also difficult to ascertain how much information about the gate was learned by probing with the straw, as the gate would move to at least some degree when pushed, which should have indicated that the gate was not immoveable. Several authors have pointed to the failure to integrate or detect potential relationships between objects and their affordances may be what makes tool use challenging 66 , 67 . Similarly, the hook bending task requires perceiving spatial relations that do not yet exist, in this case of an entirely new tool. It would be beneficial for future work to explore the potential link between affordance learning and prospective perception, especially with different tasks that have no components that can be solved without a deeper understanding of the relations between objects. It would also be informative to look into the relationship of bimanual coordination and the development of the corpus callosum with the ability to succeed in and innovate concurrent tool use tasks.

Our research confirms that the innovation gap in children exists beyond tool reshaping tasks. We suggest that prospective perception of spatial relations may be the limiting factor for children’s innovative abilities, explaining why children can still readily learn the task after social demonstration. As with very young children that can only succeed in pulling a spatially distant object with a rod once they have been shown the possible physical relationship by an adult, older children also struggle with multiple spatial relations in complex tooling tasks until they have been revealed. This is not necessarily a drawback, as learning tool use socially at a young age may be more efficient and productive than completely “reinventing the wheel” 1 , 8 . Other research has shown that similarly aged children struggle with considering the near future, such as the representation of mutually exclusive possibilities 68 , 69 , 70 , 71 , and it has been suggested that foresight may play a role in innovation 72 . It is premature to do more than speculate whether these phenomena are related without more empirical support, but it is telling that in both cases young children struggle with conceiving of yet non-existent future possibilities. The innovation gap may not simply be a failure of complex problem solving, but a failure of imagination.

Participants

Children were recruited from two primary schools located in two ethnically diverse areas in Birmingham, England, serving low- and middle- income families. The final sample consisted of 98 children (49 males, 48 females, 1 unspecified) between the ages of four and nine (range: 55 mo.–113 mo.) from three school years (5-year-old group: n  = 33, M  = 60 mo.; 7-year-old group: n  = 32, M  = 81 mo.; 9-year-old group: n  = 33, M  = 108 mo.). This excludes the data for 7 children that solved the puzzle box in an unintended manner on both conditions (e.g., throwing objects, shaking the box etc.).

Ethical approval was granted by the University of Birmingham, UK, STEM Ethical Review Committee, and research was conducted in accordance with all relevant guidelines and regulations. Informed consent was obtained from each participant’s parent or legal guardian prior to participation in the study.

The “sticker slide” is a Plexiglas box with a half of a pipe on a ramp and a plinth where a prize ball (a clear plastic ball that could be opened to obtain a sticker) can be placed. There is a wide gap in the Plexiglas at the bottom of the ramp for the ball to exit the pipe, and a small hole in line with the plinth, wide enough only to fit a slim tool. Blocking the path of the pipe is a hinged gate, which can be pushed open with a sturdy tool. In the concurrent version of the task, a piece of sponge was placed on the wall to prevent the gate from staying open without consistent pressure from the tool.

The tools provided for the experiment consisted of a thin flimsy tool (a third of a plastic straw), and a short sturdy tool (a thick wooden dowel with a cross piece attached to one end), in order to prevent the dowel from being pushed completely into the bottom hole of the apparatus. This crosspiece was added to the sturdy tool after the first session, as three children attempted to “throw” the dowel inside the box.

The pre-experience puzzle box was a simplified version of the sticker slide, consisting of only a ramp with a gap at the bottom, and a small hole in line with the plinth that could fit the 0.5 cm diameter wooden stick provided. The pre-experience box was a warm-up with no time limit employed to ensure that the children knew the affordance of a stick tool for reaching, that they had permission to use tools to solve the tasks, and to make them comfortable with the experimenter and the experimental situation.

Before testing, the teacher instructed the children not to tell the other children how to solve the puzzles in order to “keep the activity a fun surprise.” Only children given prior parental permission were allowed to participate. All participants were tested by a female experimenter (J.A.D.C.) in an open quiet room near the classroom and their performance video-recorded. Children were alternately assigned to group 1, which received the sequential version of the task first and the concurrent version second, and group 2, which received the concurrent version first and sequential version second.

Once the child entered the testing room, they were given a bookmark and then asked if they would like to solve some puzzles in order to obtain stickers to put on the back. The children were asked to first sit at a table holding the pre-experience puzzle box. The experimenter pointed out that there was a sticker contained in the clear prize ball sitting on the platform. The child was asked if they thought they could retrieve the ball, and informed that they could use anything on the table to do so. They were further instructed not to take off the lid or try to hit the box. Children that initially failed to solve the task were given a “hint,” either that they should try the stick or that there was a hole at the top, depending on their difficulty with the task. The 5-year-old group more often needed a hint (64%, 21/33), compared to the two older groups (7-year-olds, 19%, 6/32; 9-year-olds, 15%, 5/33).

The child was then asked if they would like to try another puzzle. They were asked to sit at a different table with the testing box. The experimenter repeated the instructions from the pre-experience. If, during a trial, the child appeared to give up or stated that they could not do it, then they were asked if they would like to keep trying. If they answered no, the trial ended. Otherwise, a trial ended after the child solved it or three minutes elapsed. The next trial began by the experimenter informing the child that they were going to make one little change, and asked if the child wanted to try again. The experimenter then either removed the sponge that kept the gate from staying open (for the sequential version) or added the sponge (for the concurrent version), and the child was told they could start again.

At the end of the testing session, children that could not solve either the sequential or concurrent trials were told that the experimenter would show them how to solve it and then given a full demonstration, and asked if they wanted to give it another try. If they still struggled, the experimenter either gave them a hint if they had figured out the function of both tools and were close to solving it (usually to apply more pressure to keep the gate open) or helped them to solve the puzzle by assisting with either the dowel or straw (whichever was causing the child more problems). In this way, all children who participated received all of the sticker rewards.

We recorded several outcome variables, including whether the child succeeded or failed in each condition, how long it took them to solve the puzzle (from coming in contact with the tools until the ball was dislodged from the plinth), how long it took the child to dislodge the ball from the plinth (once first contact was made with the prize ball), whether the child needed help in the pre-experience, or whether after the demonstration the child required a hint or physical help to solve the puzzle. After a suggestion by a reviewer, we had the videos re-coded to determine how many tools the child used to try and solve the puzzle, which holes they interacted with (top/bottom), whether they pushed the gate open, and which children tried concurrent tool use but did not succeed within the time limit.

All analyses were conducted in R version 4.1.1 73 with an alpha level of 0.05. Generalized linear mixed models (GLMMs) were run using the lmer/glmer functions of the lme4 package version 1.1–27.1 74 . Semi-partial R 2 were calculated using the r2glmm package version 0.1.2 75 . Variance inflation factors (VIFs) were calculated using the car package version 3.1–2 76 . The remaining model diagnostics and confidence intervals were estimated with functions kindly provided by Roger Mundry.

Data availability

All data and analyses are freely available at the Open Science Framework (OSF): https://osf.io/hrmx4/ .

Beck, S. R., Chappell, J., Apperly, I. A. & Cutting, N. Tool innovation may be a critical limiting step for the establishment of a rich tool-using culture: A perspective from child development. Behav. Brain Sci. 35 , 220–221 (2012).

Article   PubMed   Google Scholar  

Bates, E., Carlson-Luden, V. & Bretherton, I. Perceptual aspects of tool using in infancy. Infant Behav. Dev. 3 , 127–140 (1980).

Article   Google Scholar  

Brown, A. L. Domain-specific principles affect learning and transfer in children. Cogn. Sci. 14 , 107–133 (1990).

Chen, Z., Siegler, R. S. & Daehler, M. W. Across the great divide: Bridging the gap between understanding of toddlers’ and older children’s thinking. Child Dev. 65 , v–96 (2000).

Google Scholar  

Rat-Fischer, L., O’Regan, J. K. & Fagard, J. The emergence of tool use during the second year of life. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 113 , 440–446 (2012).

van Leeuwen, L., Smitsman, A. & van Leeuwen, C. Affordances, perceptual complexity, and the development of tool use. J. Exp. Child Psychol: Hum. Percept. Perform. 20, 174–191 (1994).

Weir, A. A. S., Chappell, J. & Kacelnik, A. Shaping of hooks in New Caledonian crows. Science 297 , 981–981 (2002).

Article   CAS   PubMed   Google Scholar  

Beck, S. R., Apperly, I. A., Chappell, J., Guthrie, C. & Cutting, N. Making tools isn’t child’s play. Cognition 119 , 301–306 (2011).

Cutting, N., Apperly, I. A. & Beck, S. R. Why do children lack the flexibility to innovate tools?. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 109 , 497–511 (2011).

Lew-Levy, S., Pope, S. M., Haun, D. B. M., Kline, M. A. & Broesch, T. Out of the empirical box: A mixed-methods study of tool innovation among Congolese BaYaka forager and Bondongo fisher–farmer children. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 211 , 105223 (2021).

Chappell, J., Cutting, N., Apperly, I. A. & Beck, S. R. The development of tool manufacture in humans: what helps young children make innovative tools? Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond., B, Biol. Sci. 368, 20120409 (2013).

Sheridan, K. M., Konopasky, A. W., Kirkwood, S. & Defeyter, M. A. The effects of environment and ownership on children’s innovation of tools and tool material selection. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 371 , 20150191 (2016).

Article   PubMed   PubMed Central   Google Scholar  

Beck, S. R. et al. Is tool-making knowledge robust over time and across problems? Front. Psychol. (2014).

Rawlings, B. S., Flynn, E. G. & Kendal, R. L. Personality predicts innovation and social learning in children: Implications for cultural evolution. Dev. Sci. 25 , e13153 (2022).

Henrich, J., Heine, S. J. & Norenzayan, A. The weirdest people in the world?. Behav Brain Sci 33 , 61–83 (2010).

Neldner, K., Mushin, I. & Nielsen, M. Young children’s tool innovation across culture: Affordance visibility matters. Cognition 168 , 335–343 (2017).

Nielsen, M., Tomaselli, K., Mushin, I. & Whiten, A. Exploring tool innovation: A comparison of Western and Bushman children. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 126 , 384–394 (2014).

Beck, S. R., Williams, C., Cutting, N., Apperly, I. A. & Chappell, J. Individual differences in children’s innovative problem-solving are not predicted by divergent thinking or executive functions. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 371 , 20150190 (2016).

Cutting, N., Apperly, I. A., Chappell, J. & Beck, S. R. The puzzling difficulty of tool innovation: Why can’t children piece their knowledge together?. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 125 , 110–117 (2014).

Gönül, G., Takmaz, E. K., Hohenberger, A. & Corballis, M. The cognitive ontogeny of tool making in children: The role of inhibition and hierarchical structuring. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 173 , 222–238 (2018).

Gönül, G., Hohenberger, A., Corballis, M. & Henderson, A. M. E. Joint and individual tool making in preschoolers: From social to cognitive processes. Cogn. Dev. 28 , 1037–1053 (2019).

Whalley, C. L., Cutting, N. & Beck, S. R. The effect of prior experience on children’s tool innovation. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 161 , 81–94 (2017).

Cutting, N., Apperly, I. A., Chappell, J. & Beck, S. R. Is tool modification more difficult than innovation?. Cogn. Dev. 52 , 100811 (2019).

Breyel, S. & Pauen, S. The beginnings of tool innovation in human ontogeny: How three- to five-year-olds solve the vertical and horizontal tube task. Cogn. Dev. 58 , 101049 (2021).

Cutting, N. Children’s tool making: From innovation to manufacture. PhD thesis, University of Birmingham, 2013.

Voigt, B., Pauen, S. & Bechtel-Kuehne, S. Getting the mouse out of the box: Tool innovation in preschoolers. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 184 , 65–81 (2019).

Gönül, G., Takmaz, E. & Hohenberger, A. Preschool children’s use of perceptual-motor knowledge and hierarchical representational skills for tool making. Acta Psychol. 220 , 103415 (2021).

Fragaszy, D. M. & Mangalam, M. in Adv. Study Behav. 50, 177–241 (2018)

Arbib, M. A., Bonaiuto, J. B., Jacobs, S. & Frey, S. H. Tool use and the distalization of the end-effector. Psychol. Res. 73 , 441–462 (2009).

Colbourne, J. A. D., Auersperg, A. M. I., Lambert, M. L., Huber, L. & Völter, C. J. Extending the reach of tooling theory: A neurocognitive and phylogenetic perspective. Top. Cogn. Sci. 13 , 548–572 (2021).

Jackson, G. M., Jackson, S. R. & Kritikos, A. Attention for action: Coordinating bimanual reach-to-grasp movements. Br. J. Psychol. 90 , 247–270 (1999).

Bingham, G. P., Hughes, K. & Mon-Williams, M. The coordination patterns observed when two hands reach-to-grasp separate objects. Exp. Brain Res. 184 , 283–293 (2008).

Riek, S., Tresilian, J. R., Mon-Williams, M., Coppard, V. L. & Carson, R. G. Bimanual aiming and overt attention: One law for two hands. Exp. Brain Res. 153 , 59–75 (2003).

Mason, A. H., Bruyn, J. L. & Lazarus, J.-A.C. Bimanual coordination in children: Manipulation of object distance. Exp. Brain Res. 231 , 153–164 (2013).

Schielzeth, H. Simple means to improve the interpretability of regression coefficients. Methods Ecol. Evol. 1 , 103–113 (2010).

Forstmeier, W. & Schielzeth, H. Cryptic multiple hypotheses testing in linear models: Overestimated effect sizes and the winner’s curse. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 65 , 47–55 (2011).

Franz, E. A. & Fahey, S. Developmental change in interhemispheric communication: Evidence from bimanual cost. Psychol. Sci. 18 , 1030–1031 (2007).

Hughes, C. M. L. & Franz, E. A. Experience-dependent effects in unimanual and bimanual reaction time tasks in musicians. J. Mot. Behav. 39 , 3–8 (2007).

Mason, A. H., Bruyn, J. L. & Lazarus, J.-A.C. Bimanual coordination in children: Manipulation of object size. Exp. Brain Res. 201 , 797–807 (2010).

Rudisch, J., Butler, J., Izadi, H., Birtles, D. & Green, D. Developmental characteristics of disparate bimanual movement skills in typically developing children. J. Mot. Behav. 50 , 8–16 (2018).

Fagard, J., Hardy-Léger, I., Kervella, C. & Marks, A. Changes in interhemispheric transfer rate and the development of bimanual coordination during childhood. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 80 , 1–22 (2001).

Thompson, P. M. et al. Growth patterns in the developing brain detected by using continuum mechanical tensor maps. Nature 404 , 190–193 (2000).

Article   ADS   CAS   PubMed   Google Scholar  

Westerhausen, R. et al. Structural and functional reorganization of the corpus callosum between the Age of 6 and 8 years. Cereb. Cortex 21 , 1012–1017 (2011).

Chicoine, A., Lassonde, M. & Proteau, L. Developmental aspects of sensorimotor integration. Dev. Neuropsychol. 8 , 381–394 (1992).

Serrien, D. J., Sovijärvi-Spapé, M. M. & Rana, G. Developmental changes in motor control: Insights from bimanual coordination. Dev. Psychol. 50 , 316–323 (2014).

Matheson, E. A study of problem solving behavior in pre-school children. Child Dev. 2 , 242 (1931).

Metevier, C. M. Tool-using in rhesus monkeys and 36-month-old children: Acquisition, comprehension, and individual differences. PhD thesis, University of Massachusetts Amherst, (2006).

Reindl, E., Tennie, C., Apperly, I. A., Lugosi, Z. & Beck, S. R. Young children spontaneously invent three different types of associative tool use behaviour. Evol. Hum. Sci. (2022).

Remigereau, C. et al. Involvement of Technical Reasoning More Than Functional Knowledge in Development of Tool Use in Childhood. Front. Psychol. 7, (2016).

Shumaker, R. W., Walkup, K. R. & Beck, B. B. Animal Tool Behavior: The Use and Manufacture of Tools by Animals . (Johns Hopkins University Press, 2011).

Sugiyama, Y. Social tradition and the use of tool-composites by wild chimpanzees. Evol. Anthropol. Issues, News, and Rev. 6, 23–27 (1997).

Boesch, C., Head, J. & Robbins, M. M. Complex tool sets for honey extraction among chimpanzees in Loango National Park. Gabon. J. Hum. Evol. 56 , 560–569 (2009).

Martin-Ordas, G., Schumacher, L. & Call, J. Sequential tool use in great apes. PLoS One 7 , e52074 (2012).

Article   ADS   CAS   PubMed   PubMed Central   Google Scholar  

Mulcahy, N. J., Call, J. & Dunbar, R. I. M. Gorillas (Gorilla gorilla) and orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus) encode relevant problem features in a tool-using task. J. Comp. Psychol. 119 , 23–32 (2005).

Sabbatini, G. et al. Sequential use of rigid and pliable tools in tufted capuchin monkeys (Sapajus spp.). Anim. Behav. 87, 213–220 (2014).

Osuna-Mascaró, A. J. et al. Flexible tool set transport in Goffin’s cockatoos. Curr. Biol. 33 , 849-857.e4 (2023).

Taylor, A. H., Hunt, G. R., Holzhaider, J. C. & Gray, R. D. Spontaneous metatool use by New Caledonian crows. Curr. Biol. 17 , 1504–1507 (2007).

Bird, C. D. & Emery, N. J. Insightful problem solving and creative tool modification by captive nontool-using rooks. PNAS 106 , 10370–10375 (2009).

Connolly, K. & Dalgleish, M. The emergence of a tool-using skill in infancy. Develop. Psychol. 25 , 912 (1989).

Kahrs, B. A., Jung, W. P. & Lockman, J. J. When does tool use become distinctively human? Hammering in young children. Child Dev. 85 , 1050–1061 (2013).

Reindl, E., Beck, S. R., Apperly, I. A. & Tennie, C. Young children spontaneously invent wild great apes’ tool-use behaviours. Proc. R. Soc. B 283 , 20152402 (2016).

Article   CAS   PubMed   PubMed Central   Google Scholar  

Neldner, K. et al. A cross-cultural investigation of young children’s spontaneous invention of tool use behaviours. R. Soc. Open Sci. 7 , 192240 (2020).

Article   ADS   PubMed   PubMed Central   Google Scholar  

Falótico, T. et al. Analysis of sea almond ( Terminalia catappa ) cracking sites used by wild Burmese long-tailed macaques ( Macaca fascicularis aurea ). Am. J. Primatol. 79 , e22629 (2017).

Fragaszy, D., Izar, P., Visalberghi, E., Ottoni, E. B. & De Oliveira, M. G. Wild capuchin monkeys (C ebus libidinosus ) use anvils and stone pounding tools. Am. J. Primatol. 64 , 359–366 (2004).

Osuna-Mascaró, A. J., Mundry, R., Tebbich, S., Beck, S. R. & Auersperg, A. M. I. Innovative composite tool use by Goffin’s cockatoos (Cacatua goffiniana). Sci. Reports 12 , 10 (2022).

Lockman, J. J. Perception-action perspective on tool use development. Child Dev. 71 , 137–144 (2000).

Van Leeuwen, L., Smitsman, A. & Van Leeuwen, C. Affordances, perceptual complexity, and the development of tool use. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 20 , 174–191 (1994).

Beck, S. R., Robinson, E. J., Carroll, D. J. & Apperly, I. A. Children’s thinking about counterfactuals and future hypotheticals as possibilities. Child Dev. 77 , 413–426 (2006).

Redshaw, J., Leamy, T., Pincus, P. & Suddendorf, T. Young children’s capacity to imagine and prepare for certain and uncertain future outcomes. PLOS ONE 13 , e0202606 (2018).

Redshaw, J. et al. Young children from three diverse cultures spontaneously and consistently prepare for alternative future possibilities. Child Dev. 90 , 51–61 (2019).

Redshaw, J. & Suddendorf, T. Children’s and apes’ preparatory responses to two mutually exclusive possibilities. Curr. Biol. 26 , 1758–1762 (2016).

Suddendorf, T., Redshaw, J. & Bulley, A. The Invention of Tomorrow: A Natural History of Foresight . (Basic Books, 2022).

R Core Team. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. (2021). at https://www.R-project.org/ .

Bates, D., Mächler, M., Bolker, B. & Walker, S. Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. J. Stat. Softw. 67 , 1–48 (2015).

Byron C. Jaeger. r2glmm: Computes R squared for mixed (multilevel) models. (2017).

Fox, J. & Weisberg, S. An R Companion to Applied Regression . (Sage Publications, 2019).

Download references

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank the teachers, staff, and schoolchildren of St Matthews C of E primary school and St Catherine of Siena Catholic primary school for their participation and help. We would also like to thank the Thinktank Birmingham Science Museum for hosting the pilot and volunteers Elif Bastan, Yibo Xie, Ellen O’ Callaghan, and Shefali Kelkar. Thanks to Antonio Osuna-Mascaró for feedback on the manuscript draft and Sabine Tebbich for suggestions on the study design and project funding. Our appreciation to Joelle Colbourne for helping with video coding and creating the sticker slide diagram and the supplementary video. Thank you to Roger Mundry for providing statistical guidance and several of the statistical functions in R, and Remco Folkertsma for statistical advice.

This work was funded by the Vienna Science and Technology Fund (WWTF), the City of Vienna, and Ithuba Capital AG [project CS18023] and the Austrian Science Fund (FWF) [START project Y1309]. For the purpose of open access, the author has applied a CC BY public copyright licence to any author accepted manuscript version arising from this submission.

Author information

Authors and affiliations.

Comparative Cognition Unit, Messerli Institute, University of Veterinary Medicine Vienna, Veterinärplatz 1, 1210, Vienna, Austria

Jennifer A. D. Colbourne & Alice M. I. Auersperg

School of Psychology, University of Birmingham, Edgbaston, Birmingham, B15 2TT, UK

Sarah R. Beck

You can also search for this author in PubMed   Google Scholar

Contributions

S.R.B. and A.M.I.A. obtained the project funding; J.A.D.C. designed the study with the help of S.R.B. and A.M.I.A.; J.A.D.C. collected the data under the supervision of S.R.B.; J.A.D.C. analyzed the data, created the graphs and wrote the manuscript; all authors edited and approved the manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Jennifer A. D. Colbourne .

Ethics declarations

Competing interests.

The authors declare no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher's note.

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Supplementary Video 1.

Supplementary Information 1.

Rights and permissions.

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ .

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article.

Colbourne, J.A.D., Auersperg, A.M.I. & Beck, S.R. Children’s limited tooling ability in a novel concurrent tool use task supports the innovation gap. Sci Rep 14 , 21374 (2024). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-024-71686-8

Download citation

Received : 14 February 2024

Accepted : 30 August 2024

Published : 13 September 2024

DOI : https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-024-71686-8

Share this article

Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:

Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.

Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative

  • Associative tool use
  • Object relations
  • Spatial relations
  • Spatial reasoning

By submitting a comment you agree to abide by our Terms and Community Guidelines . If you find something abusive or that does not comply with our terms or guidelines please flag it as inappropriate.

Quick links

  • Explore articles by subject
  • Guide to authors
  • Editorial policies

Sign up for the Nature Briefing newsletter — what matters in science, free to your inbox daily.

development gap case study

IMAGES

  1. Development Gap case study revision sheets

    development gap case study

  2. (PDF) Mine the (Development) Gap Case Study-1 FINAL (Compressed

    development gap case study

  3. The Development Gap

    development gap case study

  4. (PDF) Development in the gap: A case study of the emerging adult in

    development gap case study

  5. 8. Tourism And The Development Gap Case Study

    development gap case study

  6. The causes of the *development gap*

    development gap case study

VIDEO

  1. Development Gap, Part VIII Module I, Economics of Growth and Development

  2. Research and Development GAP GodAndPeople 8-25-2024, AL Cervantes

  3. FUNNY MEMES 😂 Study Gap Case

  4. Europe vs Asia: Why the Development Gap?

  5. Is a study Gap of up to 16 years acceptable? Finland Cook program application

  6. Predicting Consumer Tastes with Big Data at Gap Case Study Solution & Analysis

COMMENTS

  1. Strategies for reducing the development gap

    The development gap is the difference in levels of development between the richest and poorest countries in the world. Some believe the development gap between high-income countries (HICs) and low-income countries (LICs) is increasing. There are a range of different strategies that can be used to reduce the development gap.

  2. How can the growth of tourism reduce the development gap? Jamaica Case

    Tourism is one of Jamaica's top sources of revenue. The industry contributes over 50% of the country's total foreign exchange earnings (approx. US$2b). Thousands of Jamaicans work directly or indirectly in tourism. Tourism employs the second largest number of Jamaicans (approximately 200,000) directly in hotels, transport and attractions ...

  3. PDF THE DEVELOPMENT FINANCING GAP

    THE DEVELOPMENT FINANCING GAP. NOVEMBER 2023. This document is made possible by the support of the American People through the United States Agency for International Development (USAID). The contents of this document are the sole responsibility of INVEST, implemented by DAI, and do not necessarily reflect the ... CASE STUDIES. CONTENTS.

  4. Closing the sustainable development gap: A global study of goal

    The distances between the indicator values and their corresponding thresholds constitute the sustainable development gap. We then apply a cluster analysis technique to group 117 countries into six clusters that face similar challenges in closing this gap. ... Yet, the results from such studies are inconclusive (see, e.g., Coondo & Dinda, 2008 ...

  5. A review of the development divide between Global North and South

    Similarly, the Global North builds development norms (truths) becoming embedded in the public's perception of reality (power-knowledge) (Barsan Citation 2016). Hickel's argument is not a point about data, but a rich analysis to highlight how development practitioners preserve peoples' consent of the system without questioning the whole game.

  6. Reducing the Development Gap

    How can we encourage development in LICs and NEEs? Aid is money or resources given to places that need it. In an emergency, humanitarian aid can provide short term essentials like bottled water and tents to keep people alive. In the long term, development aid can help to improve the place socially and economically by improving things like healthcare and education so future generations can have ...

  7. PDF Assessing the development gap

    Assessing the development gap CASE Network Reports, No. 74 Provided in Cooperation with: Center for Social and Economic Research (CASE), Warsaw Suggested Citation: Sinitsina, Irina et al. (2007) : Assessing the development gap, CASE Network Reports, No. 74, ISBN 978-83-7178-444-6, Center for Social and Economic Research (CASE), Warsaw

  8. Scaling Private Investment to Close the Development Financing Gap: Case

    This set of five case studies shows how donors and MDBs effectively engaged private institutional investors, leading to billions in private sector investment for development goals. The document also synthesizes key insights and recommendations on ways in which development actors can build on and replicate this nucleus of success and harness the ...

  9. PDF the Development Gap

    —Please cite the work as follows: World Bank. 2014. "Bridging the Development Gap: ASEAN Equitable Development Monitor 2014" (November) World Bank, Washington, DC. License: Creative Commons Attribution CC BY 3.0 IGO. Translations —If you create a translation of this work, please add the following disclaimer along with the attribution:

  10. Closing Development Gaps: Challenges and Policy

    Download (Free: 682.66 KB) This study investigates income and nonincome development gaps that remain around the world. Closing these gaps requires policies that not only increase and sustain economic growth, but also directly close the nonincome development gaps. The authors analyze the extent to which human capital, infrastructure, financial ...

  11. Revision-GEOGRAPHY

    Benifitis of tourism. Economic. In 2014, tourism contributed to 24% of Jamaica's GDP, with this expected to rise to 32% by 2024. Income from tourism is $2 billion annually, with the taxes paid to the government contributing to reducing the development gap. Out of the 3.6 million annual visitors, 2.5 million spend $120 per day, with the ...

  12. Development needs to change. Bangladesh can show us how

    4 things to know about financing sustainable development. Since its founding in 1971, Bangladesh has emerged from overwhelming poverty to be proclaimed by The World Bank in 2020 as 'a model for poverty reduction'. It achieved the highest cumulative GDP growth globally from 2010 to 2020 and is now on course to become a developed country by 2041.

  13. PDF HOW TO FILL THE IMPLEMENTATION GAP FOR INCLUSIVE GROWTH

    decline from 19 kph on average to 11.6 kph by 2022, and the average home-work commuting time by car. would be increased from 37 minutes to 100 minutes.16. Worryingly, according to the latest ...

  14. Development Gap case study revision sheets

    Students complete the case studies with probing questions for the topic of the Development Gap. The pack includes Haiti as an LEDC that suffers from physical inequality (Haiti Earthquake), Small scale aid youth empowerment in Uganda, Medium scale aid, Action Aid in Kolkata, Large scale Aid - the Cahora Bassa dam, Mozambique and inequality in the EU.

  15. Strategies to Reduce the Development Gap

    Edexcel. Spanish. Past Papers. CIE. Spanish Language & Literature. Past Papers. Other Subjects. Revision notes on 5.1.2 Strategies to Reduce the Development Gap for the AQA GCSE Geography syllabus, written by the Geography experts at Save My Exams.

  16. Coolgeography

    GCSE - Changing Economic World. There are global variations in economic development and quality of life. Various strategies exist for reducing the global development gap. A case study of NIGERIA (a LICs or NEEs) Major changes in the economy of the UK.

  17. How can the growth of tourism reduce the development gap? Tunisia Case

    Tunisia experienced a year-on-year average growth rate of 1.14% for the time period 1976 to 2017. Since the recession in 2009, there has been an almost exponential growth in tourism in terms of the contribution of travel services to commercial service imports. Travel services (% of commercial service imports), % of commercial service imports.

  18. Development Gap Case Study

    Study Development Gap Case Study - Development Projects flashcards from Freya S's class online, or in Brainscape's iPhone or Android app. Learn faster with spaced repetition.

  19. GCSE Development Gap

    Mapping development - Different ways of classifying different parts of the world. Correlation between Development measures. The relationship between quality of life and standard of living. Envi ronmental, e conomic, Social & Political factors affecting inequalities. A case study of a natural hazard - the Haiti Earthquake. Reducing Inequalities

  20. AQA GCSE Human Geography

    Study with Quizlet and memorize flashcards containing terms like 7th September 2004, 200kph, Category 4, Saffir-Simpson scale and more. ... Social Science. Human Geography; AQA GCSE Human Geography - The Development Gap - Case Studies. Flashcards. Learn. Test. Match. Flashcards. Learn. Test. Match. Created by. henryleeds. Terms in this set (81 ...

  21. The Development Gap Case Studies Flashcards

    The Development Gap Case Studies. How do you want to study today? Flashcards. Review terms and definitions. Learn. Focus your studying with a path. Test. Take a practice test. Match. Get faster at matching terms. Created by. LCharilaou. GCSE Geography AQA A. Terms in this set (9) Hurricane Ivan- Granada.

  22. Children's limited tooling ability in a novel concurrent ...

    The innovation gap in two tool reshaping tasks. Each colour represents a study, each point represents an experimental condition, and sample size for each experimental condition is reflected in ...